From: NSA TORTURE TECHNOLOGY, NEWS and RESEARCH on
http://scienceblogs.com/notrocketscience/2009/05/electrical_stimulation_produces_feelings_of_free_will.php

Electrical stimulation produces feelings of free will
Category: Brain . Consciousness . Neuroscience
Posted on: May 7, 2009 2:05 PM, by Ed Yong

When it comes to the human brain, even the simplest of acts can be
counter-intuitive and deceptively complicated. For example, try stretching
your arm.

Nerves in the limb send messages back to your brain, but the subjective
experience you have of stretching isn't due to these signals. The feeling
that you willed your arm into motion, and the realisation that you moved it
at all, are both the result of an area at the back of your brain called the
posterior parietal cortex. This region helped to produce the intention to
move, and predicted what the movement would feel like, all before you
twitched a single muscle.

Michel Desmurget and a team of French neuroscientists arrived at this
conclusion by stimulating the brains of seven people with electrodes, while
they underwent brain surgery under local anaesthetic. When Desmurget
stimulated the parietal cortex, the patients felt a strong desire to move
their arms, hands, feet or lips, although they never actually did. Stronger
currents cast a powerful illusion, convincing the patients that they had
actually moved, even though recordings of electrical activity in their
muscles said otherwise.

But when Desmurget stimulated a different region - the premotor cortex - he
found the opposite effect. The patients moved their hands, arms or mouths
without realising it. One of them flexed his left wrist, fingers and elbow
and rotated his forearm, but was completely unaware of it. When his surgeons
asked if he felt anything, he said no. Higher currents evoked stronger
movements, but still the patients remained blissfully unaware that their
limbs and lips were budging.

These contrasting responses tell us two important things. Firstly, they tell
us that our feelings of free will originate (at least partially) in the
parietal cortex. It's the activity of these neurons that creates a sense
that we initiate actions of our own accord. Secondly, they show that the
sense of moving doesn't depend very much on actually doing so - it depends
on calculations that are made in the parietal cortex, long before the action
itself begins.

The seven patients in Desmurget's study were brain cancer patients, who were
having their tumours removed. All of them gave consent to have their brains
stimulated as part of the operations, something that's often done beforehand
to check that everything's working and minimise the risk of major
complications after the surgery.

Stimulating the brain with electrodes in this way gets around a major
problem with studying the idea of "free will" in a laboratory setting. It
allows scientists like Desmurget to carefully control their experiments,
delivering a very precise input and watching the result. The alternative
would be to give volunteers the framework of a task and get them to choose
what action to make, when to make it or whether to make it at all. Another
neuroscientists, Patrick Haggard, describes these experiments as
"unsatisfactory, even paradoxical", the equivalent of instructing people to
"have free will now!"

Electrodes help to bypass that problem, and this isn't the first time that
scientists have used them to the subjective side of movements. In 1991,
Itzhak Fried found that delivering an electric jolt to a person's
supplementary motor area (SMA) produced a strong urge to move. But unlike
the desires experienced by Desmurget's patients, these urges felt strong and
irrepressible, like they went beyond the patients' own will. With enough
current to the SMA, Fried could trigger actual movements.

Desmurget, on the other hand, could only ever produce the illusion of
movement by focusing on the parietal cortex. And his patients' descriptions
of their experiences made it very clear that they were feeling some sort of
internal intention to move, rather than feeling compelled by an external
force. Without any prompting from the researchers, they all described their
feelings with words such as "will", "desire" or "wanting to". One of the
patients said, "I felt a desire to lick my lips", after a low burst of
current. With more stimulation, he said "I moved my mouth. I talked. What
did I say?"

These results are a good fit with those of previous studies. Research on
monkeys suggests that the posterior parietal cortex contains a sort of "map
of intentions", where different areas are dedicated to planning different
groups of movements - looking, grasping, reaching, and so on. And in humans,
people with parietal cortex damage aren't aware of their intention to move.
They can tell when they start moving, but not when they actually decided to
do so.

To Haggard, the SMA and the parietal cortex are two sides of the same coin.
All the voluntary actions we do, from kicking a ball to opening a door,
eventually pass through the primary motor cortex, the final staging ground
where electrical thoughts are converted into muscular deed.

This area receives inputs from two others - the premotor cortex, which
governs movements that respond to something in the outside world, and the
SMA, which is involved in actions that we make of our own accord. The SMA
prepares commands for the actions we undertake. But it also communicates
with the parietal cortex, which predicts what it would feel like to carry
out those actions and create a sense of ownership over our own movements.

Dualist philosophers like Descartes believed that the mind and consciousness
exist outside the physical world, producing our actions by interacting with
the physical meat of our brains. The idea has become commonplace, but it's
challenged by neuroscientific studies like this one, which show that the
conscious intention to move emerges from electrical activity in neurons,
tangible objects that are all too real.

Reference: Desmurget, M., Reilly, K., Richard, N., Szathmari, A., Mottolese,
C., & Sirigu, A. (2009). Movement Intention After Parietal Cortex
Stimulation in Humans Science, 324 (5928), 811-813 DOI:
10.1126/science.1169896Haggard, P. (2009). The Sources of Human Volition
Science, 324 (5928), 731-733 DOI: 10.1126/science.1173827






.................................................................
Posted via TITANnews - Uncensored Newsgroups Access
>>>> at http://www.TitanNews.com <<<<
-=Every Newsgroup - Anonymous, UNCENSORED, BROADBAND Downloads=-